Az előadás letöltése folymat van. Kérjük, várjon

Az előadás letöltése folymat van. Kérjük, várjon

Regulation of Sleep and Wakefulness

Hasonló előadás


Az előadások a következő témára: "Regulation of Sleep and Wakefulness"— Előadás másolata:

1 Regulation of Sleep and Wakefulness

2

3 Definition of sleep rest-activity NOT= sleep-wakefulness
3/34 Definition of sleep rest-activity NOT= sleep-wakefulness general criteria of sleep lack of movements elevated sensory threshold full reversibility stereotypic posture specific resting place circadian organization homeostatic regulation: deprivation – rebound mammals (and birds) - polygraphic criteria

4 4/34 Stages in human sleep Berger 1929: arousal level is related to EEG patterns: δ, θ, α, β, later γ  Loomis 1937: 5 stages of the sleep-wakefulness – 1 W and 4 SWS Aserinsky and Kleitman 1953: discovery of paradoxical sleep related to dreaming Rechtschaffen-Kales criteria NREM1: 2-7 Hz, slow eye movements, <20 μV NREM2: spindles, K-complexes, slow waves at low amplitude NREM3: <2 Hz >75 μV waves 20-50% NREM4: <2 Hz >75 μV waves >50% REM: cortical activation, lack of muscle tone, rapid eye movements , twitches

5 Physiological variables in sleep
5/34 Physiological variables in sleep

6 6/34 REM sleep in humans

7 Why do we sleep? Because it is nighttime Because I am sleepy
7/34 Why do we sleep? Because it is nighttime circadian regulation Because I am sleepy homeostatic regulation Because the lecture is boring luxury sleep – effects of internal-external stimuli

8 Types of biological rhythms
8/34 Types of biological rhythms what do we call rhythm in a living organism? – physiological events occurring at approximately regular times internally controlled rhythms: breathing, heart beat, gut motility, brain waves, etc. externally determined rhythms: singing in certain birds, tulips, etc. rhythms controlled by an internal clock that is synchronized to the environment by Zeitgebers (synchronizing factors) – when these are missing: free-running rhythm

9 External-internal rhythms
9/34 External-internal rhythms De Mairan (1729): leaf movement of mimosa continues in darkness

10 Circadian rhythm in hamster
10/34 Circadian rhythm in hamster

11 Temperature dependency of circadian rhythms
11/34 Temperature dependency of circadian rhythms

12 12/34 Light effects circadian period (T) of diurnal and nocturnal animals change in opposite direction in constant light (LL) : Aschoff’s rule: diurnal animal: T decreases with light intensity nocturnal animal: T increases with light intensity circadian rule: diurnal animal: wake/sleep ratio increases with light intensity nocturnal animal: wake/sleep ratio decreases with light intensity the strong physiological effect of light is also shown by persistent oestrus short light impulses can change the phase of circadian rhythms

13 Phase-response curve I. (PRC)
13/34 Phase-response curve I. (PRC) Hannibal, Cell & Tissue Res. 309:73,2002

14 Master clock of daily rhythms
14/34 daily rhythms can be examined the most easily and probably they are the most important master clock was sought along the optic pathway lesioning various neuron groups two teams, independently, but simultaneously located the master clock: Stephan and Zucker, 1972 Moore and Eichler, 1972 it is the tiny, paired nucleus in the anterior hypothalamus, above the crossing of the optic tract: the nucleus suprachiasmaticus (SCN) in non-mammalian species, clock is also associated with the optic pathway

15 Publications on the SCN
15/34 Location of the SCN Publications on the SCN az SCN a látóideg kereszteződése felett helyezkedik el páratlan karrier, egyik legtöbbet vizsgált struktúra, elmúlt 10 évben évi 200 cikk

16 Effect of SCN ablation in rats
16/34 Effect of SCN ablation in rats

17 Discovery of clock genes
17/34 Discovery of clock genes 1985 – Martin Ralph – tau-mutant hamster short period in continuous dark (DD), Mendelian inheritance (20/22/24) breakthrough in 1994 using forward genetics – Vitaterna (PhD student) Clock mutant among the first 42 mice – abnormally long period, ceases in DD the mutation caused loss of a glu-rich region characteristic for bHLH type transcription factors conclusion: CLOCK is a transcription factor CLOCK also contains a PAS (Per-Arnt-Sim) domain – ability to form dimers with similar proteins

18 Clock mechanism (mammals)
18/34 Clock mechanism (mammals) B Bmal1 Clk Clock C B P Per1-3 Cry Cry1-2 P Cry degradation

19 Inputs to the SCN CTX, BF, HT, etc. raphe SCN retina IGL shell core
19/34 Inputs to the SCN CTX, BF, HT, etc. raphe 5-HT az SCN bemenetei – eltérés a core és a shell között a funkció kérdéses shell NPY core SCN GHT retina Glut PACAP IGL RHT

20 SCN activity in vivo SCN HT 20/34
évek óta végzünk kísérleteket a leideni egyetem élettani tanszékével in vivo és invitro MUA, 125 mikronos elektród, olykor egysejt aktivitás is fény szakaszban magasabb aktivitás éjjeli és nappali állatokban egyaránt, állandó sötétben tartott állatokban hasonló a helyzet szeletben is, a szeletkészítés előtti időhöz mérve patkányban a fordított aktivitás miatt rögtön látni, jó helyen van-e az elektród kisülés éjsaaka közepén 0.7, nappal közepén 1.4 Hz saját méréseinkben (9 sejt legalább 48 órán át) minden sejt oszcillátor a gének miatt, a NyP változás módosítja feltehetően a kisülést – mi szinkronizál? a sejtek GABA-t tartalmaznak, de gátlással is lehet szinkronizálni – ugyanakkor egyesek szerint nappal, mások szerint éjjel serkent a GABA a Cl gradiens megváltozása miatt, megint mások szerint mindig gátol a kisülés viszont nem kell az óra működéséhez: TTX és lidokain kérdés egyáltalán szinkronizáltak-e HT Meijer Meijer, J.H., Watanabe, K., Schaap, J., Albus, H., Détári, L.., J. Neurosci. 18(1998):9078

21 Outputs of the SCN PVN medial HT IGL other targets SCN shell core MPOA
21/34 PVN medial HT MPOA PVN, sPVN DMH, VMH IGL legfontosabb kapcsolatok kiemelni a tobozmirigy szabályozását a szimpatikus idegrendszeren keresztül endocrine neurons CRF, TRH, GnRH vegetative neurons sympathetic, parasympathetic integrating neurons other targets SCN shell core

22 Why do we sleep? Because it is nighttime Because I am sleepy
22/34 Why do we sleep? Because it is nighttime circadian regulation Because I am sleepy homeostatic regulation Because the lecture is boring luxury sleep – effects of internal-external stimuli

23 Humoral regulation of sleep
23/34 Humoral regulation of sleep closely related to homeostatic regulation something is being accumulated or used up sleep can be easily disturbed, but difficult to induce, appropriate control is a main issue two approaches: harmful effects of sleep deprivation stress is difficult to eliminate motivation to sleep is almost as strong as motivation to avoid pain - torture isolation of sleep factors following sleep deprivation during natural or experimentally evoked sleep testing prospective signal molecules normally present in our body

24 24/34 Sleep deprivation NREM deprivation: set-point of thermo-regulation increases REM deprivation: heat dissipation increases energy homeostasis becomes disturbed

25 25/34 Sleep factors Ishimori, Pieron, ~1910: dogs kept awake by forced walking for 10 days – successful sleep transfer methodological problems – repeated with positive results in goat-rat experiments deprivation is not needed for the effect – collection of human urine end result: muramyl peptide from the bacterial cell wall Monnier sleep induced by thalamic stimulation in rabbits: DSIP (9 aa-s) – the best candidate but: these are not natural sleep factors natural signal molecules: GHRH, adenosine, interleukin-1, TNFα, PGD2

26 Transfer of natural sleep
26/34 Transfer of natural sleep parabiotic animals: Matsumoto, 1972 – higher synchronity of NREM and REM sleep than between animals joined by their skin only de Andres, 1976 – transplantation of an additional head to dogs – independent sleep, 108 h survival Siamese twins – independent sleep is possible, but contradicting results exist Mukhametov, sleep in dolphins – the two hemispheres can sleep separately described in other animals as well: birds, whale, etc. – complete decussation of the visual pathway is a prerequisite

27 Unihemispheric sleep in dolphins
27/34 Unihemispheric sleep in dolphins

28 Synthesis 28/34

29 Why do we sleep? Because it is nighttime Because I am sleepy
29/34 Why do we sleep? Because it is nighttime circadian regulation Because I am sleepy homeostatic regulation Because the lecture is boring luxury sleep – effects of internal-external stimuli

30 Neuronal regulation of sleep
30/34 Neuronal regulation of sleep active hypothesis – basal state: waking Pavlov: irradiation of inhibition search for sleep centers - criteria: lesion – elimination of sleep stimulation – induction of sleep neuronal activity – correlated with sleep von Economo: encephalitis outbrake – basal forebrain midpontine pretrigeminal transsection - raphe – Jouvet, 1967 spindles - thalamus – Andersen and Anderson, 1968 passive hypothesis – basal state: sleep classical transsections of Bremer, 30’s  Moruzzi and Magoun, ARAS Shute and Lewis, 1967 – supposed ACh pathways

31 Arousing areas midline and intralaminar nuclei (glu)
31/34 Arousing areas midline and intralaminar nuclei (glu) magnocellular BF sytem (ACh, GABA, ?) posterior-lateral hypothalamus (HA, ORX) midbrain and pontine nuclei (NA, ACh, glu, 5-HT, DA)

32 Role of the basal forebrain
32/34 Role of the basal forebrain von Economo: BF-POA promotes sleep, posterior HT promotes wakefulness Sterman and Clemente lesion causes decreased or fragmented sleep stimulation – sleep (also at high frequency!) conditioned response to sounds warming, ACh crystals – sleep late 70’s, early 80’s – description of the cholinergic system  cholinergic cells disappear or shrink in Alzheimer’s disease electrical – excitotoxic – selective lesion corticopetal projection is not exclusively cholinergic SCN, thermoregulation, proximity of HT, VLPO, prefrontal cortex – high importance

33 Unilateral lesion of BF I.
33/34 Unilateral lesion of BF I. Vanderwolf, C.H., Int. Rev. Neurobiol., 30 (1988):

34 Unilateral lesion of BF II.
34/34 Unilateral lesion of BF II. scopolamine no drug before lesion after R. NB lesion modified from Buzsáki et al., J.Neurosci 8 (1988):

35

36 Berger bár voltak korábi próbálkozások állatok feltárt agykérgén (Richard Caton, 1875, Liverpool, majom és nyúl, tőle függetlenül Adolf Beck, 1891, Krakow, majd Lvov, nyúl és kutya – leírták a ritmikus oszcillációkat, az ingerek hatását, és a ritmusok függését az állat állapotától), az első, aki emberi EEG-t regisztrált a hajas fejbőrről, és leírta annak kapcsolatát az éberségi szinttel, Hans Berger volt Jenaban. az első regisztrálás saját fián 1925-ben a Siemens igen modern berendezéseivel 1929 és 1938 között 23 cikk (már akkor is fontos volt a publikációs lista)

37 Sleep stages

38 Brainstem transsections


Letölteni ppt "Regulation of Sleep and Wakefulness"

Hasonló előadás


Google Hirdetések